Anxiety and Threat-Related Attention: Cognitive-Motivational Framework and Treatment

心理学 焦虑 认知心理学 认知 认知科学 心理治疗师 发展心理学 神经科学 精神科
作者
Karin Mogg,Brendan P. Bradley
出处
期刊:Trends in Cognitive Sciences [Elsevier]
卷期号:22 (3): 225-240 被引量:158
标识
DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.001
摘要

Research in experimental psychopathology has led to the development of ABM training as a potential computer-delivered treatment for anxiety disorders. Conventional ABM threat-avoidance training encourages anxious individuals to orient attention away from threat, but has variable effects on anxiety and threat-related ABs. Differing explanations for mixed outcomes of ABM training, and theoretical controversy about the causes of anxiety and ABs, encourage the development of alternative novel ABM training methods. Development of effective attention-based treatments for anxiety would be advanced by better theoretical understanding of the cognitive processes underlying anxiety and ABs, and by using more refined and comprehensive assessments of threat-related attention and associated cognitive and neural functioning. Research in experimental psychopathology and cognitive theories of anxiety highlight threat-related attention biases (ABs) and underpin the development of a computer-delivered treatment for anxiety disorders: attention-bias modification (ABM) training. Variable effects of ABM training on anxiety and ABs generate conflicting research recommendations, novel ABM training procedures, and theoretical controversy. This article summarises an updated cognitive-motivational framework, integrating proposals from cognitive models of anxiety and attention, as well as evidence of ABs. Interactions between motivational salience-driven and goal-directed influences on multiple cognitive processes (e.g., stimulus evaluation, inhibition, switching, orienting) underlie anxiety and the variable manifestations of ABs (orienting towards and away from threat; threat-distractor interference). This theoretical analysis also considers ABM training as cognitive skill training, describes a conceptual framework for evaluating/developing novel ABM training procedures, and complements network-based research on reciprocal anxiety–cognition relationships. Research in experimental psychopathology and cognitive theories of anxiety highlight threat-related attention biases (ABs) and underpin the development of a computer-delivered treatment for anxiety disorders: attention-bias modification (ABM) training. Variable effects of ABM training on anxiety and ABs generate conflicting research recommendations, novel ABM training procedures, and theoretical controversy. This article summarises an updated cognitive-motivational framework, integrating proposals from cognitive models of anxiety and attention, as well as evidence of ABs. Interactions between motivational salience-driven and goal-directed influences on multiple cognitive processes (e.g., stimulus evaluation, inhibition, switching, orienting) underlie anxiety and the variable manifestations of ABs (orienting towards and away from threat; threat-distractor interference). This theoretical analysis also considers ABM training as cognitive skill training, describes a conceptual framework for evaluating/developing novel ABM training procedures, and complements network-based research on reciprocal anxiety–cognition relationships. Anxiety disorders are common mental-health problems that are burdensome for sufferers and health services [1Craske M.G. Stein M.B. Anxiety.Lancet. 2016; 388: 3048-3059Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (68) Google Scholar]. Treatments include medication and cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT); however, many anxiety sufferers fail to respond, or relapse [1Craske M.G. Stein M.B. Anxiety.Lancet. 2016; 388: 3048-3059Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (68) Google Scholar, 2Olatunji B.O. et al.Efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety disorders: a review of meta-analytic findings.Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 2010; 33: 557-577Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (154) Google Scholar]. Consequently, there is need for additional treatment options which are effective, low-cost, and easily delivered. One potential computer-delivered treatment is attention-bias modification (ABM) training. ABM training arose from research in experimental psychopathology (see Glossary), and draws on theory and methods in experimental cognitive psychology to examine and modify attention processes in anxiety. The original version, ABM threat-avoidance training, is based on assumptions that anxious individuals have an enduring automatic tendency to attend preferentially to threat information [3Williams M. et al.Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders.1st edn. Wiley, 1988Google Scholar], and reduction of this attention bias (AB) should reduce anxiety [4MacLeod C. et al.Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias.J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2002; 111: 107-123Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 5MacLeod C. Clarke P.J.F. The attentional bias modification approach to anxiety intervention.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2015; 3: 58-78Crossref Scopus (98) Google Scholar]. However, there is dispute about the clinical effectiveness of ABM training and the cognitive processes underlying anxiety and threat-related ABs. The purpose of this article is several-fold: to provide a brief overview of these issues, which builds on and extends our previous reviews [6Mogg K. Bradley B.P. Anxiety and attention to threat: cognitive mechanisms and treatment with attention bias modification.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 87: 76-108Crossref PubMed Scopus (44) Google Scholar, 7Mogg K. et al.Attention bias modification (ABM): review of effects of multisession ABM training on anxiety and threat-related attention in high-anxious individuals.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2017; 54: 698-717Crossref Scopus (3) Google Scholar]; to describe an updated cognitive-motivational model of anxiety and threat-related attention; to view ABM training as cognitive skill training; to apply the distinction between processes and procedures [8MacLeod C. Grafton B. Anxiety-linked attentional bias and its modification: illustrating the importance of distinguishing processes and procedures in experimental psychopathology research.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 86: 68-86Crossref PubMed Google Scholar] to this updated perspective; to describe a conceptual framework for evaluating and developing novel ABM training procedures; and to encourage network-based research evaluating reciprocal anxiety–cognition relationships. The most widely used method, ABM threat-avoidance training, typically employs a modified visual probe task to train anxious individuals to direct attention away from threat [4MacLeod C. et al.Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias.J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2002; 111: 107-123Crossref PubMed Google Scholar]. Each trial presents a threat and nonthreat cue simultaneously in different locations of a computer screen, immediately followed by a probe (e.g., a dot) which replaces one of the cues (Figure 1). Participants respond as quickly as possible to the probe. In ABM threat-avoidance training, probes are unlikely to appear in locations just vacated by threat cues. Hence, training (many hundreds of trials) encourages orienting of attention away from the location of threat. Since its introduction about 15 years ago, over 30 studies have evaluated the effects of ABM threat-avoidance training on anxiety symptoms and AB in high-anxiety individuals. Despite initial promising results, many studies report disappointing outcomes [6Mogg K. Bradley B.P. Anxiety and attention to threat: cognitive mechanisms and treatment with attention bias modification.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 87: 76-108Crossref PubMed Scopus (44) Google Scholar, 7Mogg K. et al.Attention bias modification (ABM): review of effects of multisession ABM training on anxiety and threat-related attention in high-anxious individuals.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2017; 54: 698-717Crossref Scopus (3) Google Scholar, 8MacLeod C. Grafton B. Anxiety-linked attentional bias and its modification: illustrating the importance of distinguishing processes and procedures in experimental psychopathology research.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 86: 68-86Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 9Cristea I.A. et al.Efficacy of cognitive bias modification interventions in anxiety and depression: meta-analysis.Br. J. Psychiatry. 2015; 206: 7-16Crossref PubMed Scopus (176) Google Scholar, 10Cristea I.A. et al.Practitioner review: cognitive bias modification for mental health problems in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis.J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2015; 56: 723-734Crossref PubMed Scopus (23) Google Scholar, 11Heeren A. et al.Attention bias modification for social anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2015; 40: 76-90Crossref PubMed Scopus (90) Google Scholar, 12Van Bockstaele B. et al.A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional bias on fear and anxiety.Psychol. Bull. 2014; 140: 682-721Crossref PubMed Scopus (103) Google Scholar, 13Price R.B. et al.Pooled patient-level meta-analysis of children and adults completing a computer-based anxiety intervention targeting attentional bias.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016; 50: 37-49Crossref PubMed Scopus (36) Google Scholar, 14Mogoaşe C. et al.Clinical efficacy of attentional bias modification procedures: an updated meta-analysis.J. Clin. Psychol. 2014; 70: 1133-1157Crossref PubMed Scopus (146) Google Scholar, 15Liu H. et al.Effects of cognitive bias modification on social anxiety: a meta-analysis.PLoS One. 2017; 12: 1-24Google Scholar, 16Jones E.B. Sharpe L. Cognitive bias modification: a review of meta-analyses.J. Affect. Disord. 2017; 223: 175-183Crossref PubMed Scopus (24) Google Scholar]. ABM threat-avoidance training is often no more effective in reducing anxiety than control attention training (e.g., a visual probe task in which probes are equally likely to replace threat and nonthreat cues) which combines attention task practice and threat-cue exposure, but does not encourage threat-avoidant orienting. Recent reviews and meta-analyses conclude that conventional ABM threat-avoidance training has inconsistent effects on anxiety and AB [6Mogg K. Bradley B.P. Anxiety and attention to threat: cognitive mechanisms and treatment with attention bias modification.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 87: 76-108Crossref PubMed Scopus (44) Google Scholar, 7Mogg K. et al.Attention bias modification (ABM): review of effects of multisession ABM training on anxiety and threat-related attention in high-anxious individuals.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2017; 54: 698-717Crossref Scopus (3) Google Scholar, 8MacLeod C. Grafton B. Anxiety-linked attentional bias and its modification: illustrating the importance of distinguishing processes and procedures in experimental psychopathology research.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 86: 68-86Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 9Cristea I.A. et al.Efficacy of cognitive bias modification interventions in anxiety and depression: meta-analysis.Br. J. Psychiatry. 2015; 206: 7-16Crossref PubMed Scopus (176) Google Scholar, 10Cristea I.A. et al.Practitioner review: cognitive bias modification for mental health problems in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis.J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2015; 56: 723-734Crossref PubMed Scopus (23) Google Scholar, 11Heeren A. et al.Attention bias modification for social anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2015; 40: 76-90Crossref PubMed Scopus (90) Google Scholar, 12Van Bockstaele B. et al.A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional bias on fear and anxiety.Psychol. Bull. 2014; 140: 682-721Crossref PubMed Scopus (103) Google Scholar, 13Price R.B. et al.Pooled patient-level meta-analysis of children and adults completing a computer-based anxiety intervention targeting attentional bias.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016; 50: 37-49Crossref PubMed Scopus (36) Google Scholar, 14Mogoaşe C. et al.Clinical efficacy of attentional bias modification procedures: an updated meta-analysis.J. Clin. Psychol. 2014; 70: 1133-1157Crossref PubMed Scopus (146) Google Scholar, 15Liu H. et al.Effects of cognitive bias modification on social anxiety: a meta-analysis.PLoS One. 2017; 12: 1-24Google Scholar, 16Jones E.B. Sharpe L. Cognitive bias modification: a review of meta-analyses.J. Affect. Disord. 2017; 223: 175-183Crossref PubMed Scopus (24) Google Scholar], and is therefore unsuitable as a treatment for anxiety. Other ABM training methods (considered later) may be more effective, but outcome data are preliminary. Hence, ABM training research is at a turning point, with diverse recommendations: (i) research into ABM training as a treatment for anxiety should be abandoned [9Cristea I.A. et al.Efficacy of cognitive bias modification interventions in anxiety and depression: meta-analysis.Br. J. Psychiatry. 2015; 206: 7-16Crossref PubMed Scopus (176) Google Scholar]. (ii) ABM training procedures should be improved to be more effective in training threat-avoidant attention-orienting [5MacLeod C. Clarke P.J.F. The attentional bias modification approach to anxiety intervention.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2015; 3: 58-78Crossref Scopus (98) Google Scholar, 8MacLeod C. Grafton B. Anxiety-linked attentional bias and its modification: illustrating the importance of distinguishing processes and procedures in experimental psychopathology research.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 86: 68-86Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 17Grafton B. et al.Confusing procedures with process when appraising the impact of cognitive bias modification on emotional vulnerability.Br. J. Psychiatry. 2017; 211: 266-271Crossref PubMed Scopus (1) Google Scholar]. (iii) The theoretical premise of ABM training should be reconsidered, and ABM training methods revised to target multiple processes underlying anxiety and ABs [6Mogg K. Bradley B.P. Anxiety and attention to threat: cognitive mechanisms and treatment with attention bias modification.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 87: 76-108Crossref PubMed Scopus (44) Google Scholar, 7Mogg K. et al.Attention bias modification (ABM): review of effects of multisession ABM training on anxiety and threat-related attention in high-anxious individuals.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2017; 54: 698-717Crossref Scopus (3) Google Scholar, 18Zvielli A. et al.Targeting biased emotional attention to threat as a dynamic process in time: Attention Feedback Awareness and Control Training (A-FACT).Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2016; 4: 287-298Crossref Scopus (6) Google Scholar, 19Amir I. et al.(De)Coupling of our eyes and our mind's eye: a dynamic process perspective on attentional bias.Emotion. 2016; 53: 1689-1699Google Scholar]. This article considers evidence-based cognitive perspectives of anxiety, threat-related attention, and cognitive skill training which may guide future research. To improve the efficacy of ABM training, it is helpful to consider why conventional ABM threat-avoidance training does not consistently reduce anxiety as expected. An important distinction is between target processes and training procedures [8MacLeod C. Grafton B. Anxiety-linked attentional bias and its modification: illustrating the importance of distinguishing processes and procedures in experimental psychopathology research.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 86: 68-86Crossref PubMed Google Scholar]. Poor outcomes may be explained by deficient training procedures: if ABM threat-avoidance training fails to modify its target process (i.e., reduce pre-existing AB in orienting towards threat), this could explain its failure to reduce anxiety [5MacLeod C. Clarke P.J.F. The attentional bias modification approach to anxiety intervention.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2015; 3: 58-78Crossref Scopus (98) Google Scholar, 8MacLeod C. Grafton B. Anxiety-linked attentional bias and its modification: illustrating the importance of distinguishing processes and procedures in experimental psychopathology research.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 86: 68-86Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 16Jones E.B. Sharpe L. Cognitive bias modification: a review of meta-analyses.J. Affect. Disord. 2017; 223: 175-183Crossref PubMed Scopus (24) Google Scholar, 17Grafton B. et al.Confusing procedures with process when appraising the impact of cognitive bias modification on emotional vulnerability.Br. J. Psychiatry. 2017; 211: 266-271Crossref PubMed Scopus (1) Google Scholar]. For example, ABM threat-avoidance training is less effective when delivered at home than in laboratory/clinic settings, possibly because greater distractions at home impair learning [8MacLeod C. Grafton B. Anxiety-linked attentional bias and its modification: illustrating the importance of distinguishing processes and procedures in experimental psychopathology research.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 86: 68-86Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 13Price R.B. et al.Pooled patient-level meta-analysis of children and adults completing a computer-based anxiety intervention targeting attentional bias.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016; 50: 37-49Crossref PubMed Scopus (36) Google Scholar]. ABM threat-avoidance training is also repetitious and boring, which may reduce treatment compliance [20Dennis T.A. O'Toole L.J. Mental health on the go.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2014; 2: 576-590Crossref PubMed Scopus (40) Google Scholar, 21Amir N. et al.A pilot study of an adaptive, idiographic, and multi-component attention bias modification program for social anxiety disorder.Cogn. Ther. Res. 2016; 40: 661-671Crossref PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar]. The implication would be to improve ABM threat-avoidance training procedures, for example by reducing distraction and by making training more engaging [8MacLeod C. Grafton B. Anxiety-linked attentional bias and its modification: illustrating the importance of distinguishing processes and procedures in experimental psychopathology research.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 86: 68-86Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 20Dennis T.A. O'Toole L.J. Mental health on the go.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2014; 2: 576-590Crossref PubMed Scopus (40) Google Scholar, 21Amir N. et al.A pilot study of an adaptive, idiographic, and multi-component attention bias modification program for social anxiety disorder.Cogn. Ther. Res. 2016; 40: 661-671Crossref PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar]. Another explanation for poor outcomes concerns the target process: if AB in orienting towards threat does not play a strong causal role in anxiety, this may explain the inconsistent effects of ABM threat-avoidance training [6Mogg K. Bradley B.P. Anxiety and attention to threat: cognitive mechanisms and treatment with attention bias modification.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 87: 76-108Crossref PubMed Scopus (44) Google Scholar, 11Heeren A. et al.Attention bias modification for social anxiety: a systematic review and meta-analysis.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2015; 40: 76-90Crossref PubMed Scopus (90) Google Scholar, 12Van Bockstaele B. et al.A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional bias on fear and anxiety.Psychol. Bull. 2014; 140: 682-721Crossref PubMed Scopus (103) Google Scholar, 18Zvielli A. et al.Targeting biased emotional attention to threat as a dynamic process in time: Attention Feedback Awareness and Control Training (A-FACT).Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2016; 4: 287-298Crossref Scopus (6) Google Scholar]. Other cognitive processes may underpin both anxiety and ABs to threat. If so, training procedures should be modified to target them. Numerous cognitive models of anxiety and threat-related ABs have emerged in the past 30 years (e.g., [3Williams M. et al.Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders.1st edn. Wiley, 1988Google Scholar, 6Mogg K. Bradley B.P. Anxiety and attention to threat: cognitive mechanisms and treatment with attention bias modification.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 87: 76-108Crossref PubMed Scopus (44) Google Scholar, 22Öhman A. Fear and anxiety as emotional phenomena: clinical phenomenology, evolutionary perspectives, and information-processing mechanisms.in: Lewis M. Haviland J. Handbook of Emotions. The Guilford Press, 1993: 511-536Google Scholar, 23Beck A.T. Clark D.A. An information processing model of anxiety: automatic and strategic processes.Behav. Res. Ther. 1997; 35: 49-58Crossref PubMed Scopus (579) Google Scholar, 24Derryberry D. Rothbart M.K. Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament.Dev. Psychopathol. 1997; 9: 633-652Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 25Cisler J.M. Koster E.H.W. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010; 30: 203-216Crossref PubMed Scopus (625) Google Scholar, 26Eysenck M.W. et al.Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory.Emotion. 2007; 7: 336-353Crossref PubMed Scopus (1674) Google Scholar, 27Lonigan C.J. et al.Temperament, anxiety, and the processing of threat-relevant stimuli.J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2004; 33: 8-20Crossref PubMed Scopus (177) Google Scholar, 28Bar-Haim Y. et al.Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study.Psychol. Bull. 2007; 133: 1-24Crossref PubMed Scopus (1748) Google Scholar, 29Mogg K. Bradley B.P. A cognitive motivational analysis of anxiety.Behav. Res. Ther. 1998; 36: 809-848Crossref PubMed Scopus (1037) Google Scholar, 30Mathews A. Mackintosh B. A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety.Cogn. Res. Ther. 1998; 22: 539-560Crossref Scopus (430) Google Scholar, 31Williams M. et al.Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders.2nd edn. Wiley, 1997Google Scholar, 32Waters A.M. Craske M.G. Towards a cognitive-learning formulation of youth anxiety: a narrative review of theory and evidence and implications for treatment.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016; 50: 50-66Crossref PubMed Scopus (3) Google Scholar]; reviewed in [6Mogg K. Bradley B.P. Anxiety and attention to threat: cognitive mechanisms and treatment with attention bias modification.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 87: 76-108Crossref PubMed Scopus (44) Google Scholar]). We focus here on two perspectives with differing implications for ABM training: one perspective proposes that anxiety is caused by automatic AB towards threat, which can be targeted by conventional ABM threat-avoidance training; the other proposes that anxiety and ABs are caused by multiple cognitive processes, which can be targeted more effectively by multicomponent ABM training procedures. According to early cognitive views of anxiety, high-anxiety individuals have an enduring AB towards threat, whereas low-anxiety individuals are threat-avoidant [3Williams M. et al.Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders.1st edn. Wiley, 1988Google Scholar]. AB towards threat is conceptualised as a stable trait-like characteristic which plays a causal role in anxiety; hence, interventions that reduce AB to threat should reduce anxiety [5MacLeod C. Clarke P.J.F. The attentional bias modification approach to anxiety intervention.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2015; 3: 58-78Crossref Scopus (98) Google Scholar, 33Bar-Haim Y. Research review: attention bias modification (ABM): a novel treatment for anxiety disorders.J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2010; 51: 859-870Crossref PubMed Scopus (239) Google Scholar]. Empirical research indicates that anxiety-related AB towards threat can operate automatically, outside awareness [6Mogg K. Bradley B.P. Anxiety and attention to threat: cognitive mechanisms and treatment with attention bias modification.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 87: 76-108Crossref PubMed Scopus (44) Google Scholar, 25Cisler J.M. Koster E.H.W. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010; 30: 203-216Crossref PubMed Scopus (625) Google Scholar, 34Hedger N. et al.Are visual threats prioritized without awareness? A critical review and meta-analysis involving 3 behavioral paradigms and 2696 observers.Psychol. Bull. 2016; 142: 1-123Crossref PubMed Scopus (10) Google Scholar, 35Teachman B.A. et al.Automaticity in anxiety disorders and major depressive disorder.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2012; 32: 575-603Crossref PubMed Scopus (60) Google Scholar]. Thus, conventional ABM threat-avoidance training uses implicit training procedures (i.e., training without awareness of what is being taught) and relies on repeated practice on hundreds of trials to reduce automatic attention-orienting to threat (participants are typically not informed that the training goal is to induce threat avoidance) [5MacLeod C. Clarke P.J.F. The attentional bias modification approach to anxiety intervention.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2015; 3: 58-78Crossref Scopus (98) Google Scholar, 33Bar-Haim Y. Research review: attention bias modification (ABM): a novel treatment for anxiety disorders.J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2010; 51: 859-870Crossref PubMed Scopus (239) Google Scholar]. Empirical evidence increasingly indicates that anxious individuals do not consistently exhibit AB towards threat, and sometimes show no AB or threat avoidance [6Mogg K. Bradley B.P. Anxiety and attention to threat: cognitive mechanisms and treatment with attention bias modification.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 87: 76-108Crossref PubMed Scopus (44) Google Scholar, 12Van Bockstaele B. et al.A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional bias on fear and anxiety.Psychol. Bull. 2014; 140: 682-721Crossref PubMed Scopus (103) Google Scholar, 18Zvielli A. et al.Targeting biased emotional attention to threat as a dynamic process in time: Attention Feedback Awareness and Control Training (A-FACT).Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2016; 4: 287-298Crossref Scopus (6) Google Scholar, 25Cisler J.M. Koster E.H.W. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010; 30: 203-216Crossref PubMed Scopus (625) Google Scholar, 31Williams M. et al.Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders.2nd edn. Wiley, 1997Google Scholar, 36Shechner T. Bar-Haim Y. Threat monitoring and attention-bias modification in anxiety and stress-related disorders.Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2016; 25: 431-437Crossref Scopus (3) Google Scholar, 37Salum G.A. et al.Threat bias in attention orienting: evidence of specificity in a large community-based study.Psychol. Med. 2013; 43: 733-745Crossref PubMed Scopus (70) Google Scholar, 38Yiend J. et al.Mechanisms of selective attention in generalized anxiety disorder.Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2015; 3: 758-771Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 39Waters A.M. et al.Biased attention to threat in paediatric anxiety disorders (generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, specific phobia, separation anxiety disorder) as a function of 'distress' versus 'fear' diagnostic categorization.Psychol. Med. 2014; 44: 607-616Crossref PubMed Google Scholar]. This may be explained by cognitive models of anxiety which propose that anxiety and ABs depend on multiple processes, including automatic salience-driven (also known as bottom-up, stimulus-driven) and goal-directed cognitive control (also known as top-down, executive control) processes [6Mogg K. Bradley B.P. Anxiety and attention to threat: cognitive mechanisms and treatment with attention bias modification.Behav. Res. Ther. 2016; 87: 76-108Crossref PubMed Scopus (44) Google Scholar, 23Beck A.T. Clark D.A. An information processing model of anxiety: automatic and strategic processes.Behav. Res. Ther. 1997; 35: 49-58Crossref PubMed Scopus (579) Google Scholar, 24Derryberry D. Rothbart M.K. Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament.Dev. Psychopathol. 1997; 9: 633-652Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 25Cisler J.M. Koster E.H.W. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: an integrative review.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010; 30: 203-216Crossref PubMed Scopus (625) Google Scholar, 26Eysenck M.W. et al.Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory.Emotion. 2007; 7: 336-353Crossref PubMed Scopus (1674) Google Scholar, 27Lonigan C.J. et al.Temperament, anxiety, and the processing of threat-relevant stimuli.J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2004; 33: 8-20Crossref PubMed Scopus (177) Google Scholar, 28Bar-Haim Y. et al.Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: a meta-analytic study.Psychol. Bull. 2007; 133: 1-24Crossref PubMed Scopus (1748) Google Scholar, 29Mogg K. Bradley B.P. A cognitive motivational analysis of anxiety.Behav. Res. Ther. 1998; 36: 809-848Crossref PubMed Scopus (1037) Google Scholar, 30Mathews A. Mackintosh B. A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety.Cogn. Res. Ther. 1998; 22: 539-560Crossref Scopus (430) Google Scholar, 31Williams M. et al.Cognitive Psychology and Emotional Disorders.2nd edn. Wiley, 1997Google Scholar, 32Waters A.M. Craske M.G. Towards a cognitive-learning formulation of youth anxiety: a narrative review of theory and evidence and implications for treatment.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2016; 50: 50-66Crossref PubMed Scopus (3) Google Scholar]. These models have common themes which overlap with theories of attention and cognitive control [40Petersen S.E. Posner M.I. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after.Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012; 35: 73-89Crossref PubMed Scopus (925) Google Scholar, 41Duncan J. The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010; 14: 172-179Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (563) Google Scholar, 42Diamond A. Executive functions.Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013; 64: 135-168Crossref PubMed Scopus (1018) Google Scholar, 43Corbetta M. Shulman G.L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain.Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002; 3: 215-229Crossref Google Scholar, 44Miyake A. et al.The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex 'frontal lobe' tasks: a latent variable analysis.Cogn. Psychol. 2000; 41: 49-100Crossref PubMed Scopus (5667) Google

科研通智能强力驱动
Strongly Powered by AbleSci AI
科研通是完全免费的文献互助平台,具备全网最快的应助速度,最高的求助完成率。 对每一个文献求助,科研通都将尽心尽力,给求助人一个满意的交代。
实时播报
1秒前
2秒前
落寞丹萱发布了新的文献求助10
2秒前
喝汤一样发布了新的文献求助10
3秒前
lca507发布了新的文献求助10
4秒前
112233发布了新的文献求助10
5秒前
qing完成签到,获得积分10
6秒前
9秒前
9秒前
李爱国应助墩子采纳,获得10
9秒前
莉莉发布了新的文献求助10
9秒前
12秒前
12秒前
所所应助Nero采纳,获得10
12秒前
13秒前
耍酷如柏完成签到,获得积分10
13秒前
bailifentai发布了新的文献求助10
13秒前
科研通AI5应助yaxin采纳,获得10
14秒前
小蘑菇应助爱听歌笑寒采纳,获得10
14秒前
14秒前
明理的之云完成签到,获得积分10
15秒前
科研通AI2S应助莉莉采纳,获得10
15秒前
华仔应助虫子采纳,获得10
16秒前
16秒前
17秒前
17秒前
18秒前
星辰大海应助鳗鱼没采纳,获得10
18秒前
yuhaha发布了新的文献求助10
18秒前
19秒前
FashionBoy应助灵境采纳,获得10
19秒前
19秒前
罗氏集团发布了新的文献求助10
20秒前
无花果应助ff采纳,获得10
20秒前
20秒前
20秒前
谨言发布了新的文献求助10
22秒前
22秒前
23秒前
gc发布了新的文献求助10
23秒前
高分求助中
Continuum Thermodynamics and Material Modelling 4000
Production Logging: Theoretical and Interpretive Elements 2700
Les Mantodea de Guyane Insecta, Polyneoptera 1000
Unseen Mendieta: The Unpublished Works of Ana Mendieta 1000
El viaje de una vida: Memorias de María Lecea 800
Theory of Block Polymer Self-Assembly 750
Luis Lacasa - Sobre esto y aquello 700
热门求助领域 (近24小时)
化学 材料科学 生物 医学 工程类 有机化学 生物化学 物理 纳米技术 计算机科学 内科学 化学工程 复合材料 基因 遗传学 物理化学 催化作用 量子力学 光电子学 冶金
热门帖子
关注 科研通微信公众号,转发送积分 3514705
求助须知:如何正确求助?哪些是违规求助? 3097068
关于积分的说明 9233846
捐赠科研通 2792049
什么是DOI,文献DOI怎么找? 1532249
邀请新用户注册赠送积分活动 711846
科研通“疑难数据库(出版商)”最低求助积分说明 707032