Developments in Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation to Study Human Cognition

磁刺激 心理学 认知 神经科学 刺激 认知科学 认知心理学
作者
Eva Feredoes
出处
期刊:Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience [The MIT Press]
卷期号:35 (1): 6-10 被引量:1
标识
DOI:10.1162/jocn_a_01923
摘要

The study of human cognition through neuroscience has benefitted from a constant development and transformation of associated technology and analysis. Taking working memory (WM) as an example, and also as the area in which Mark Stokes has demonstrated this, it is a field that has often found itself right at the forefront of cognitive neuroscience, with a fascinating progression of theoretical insight and development as a consequence (see D'Esposito and Postle [2014] and Postle [2006] for thorough reviews of the field). Often, this innovation and discovery has produced theoretical upheaval, such as understanding the relationship/dependence/separation between attention and WM (Nobre & Stokes, 2011), challenging the limited capacity of WM (Bays & Husain, 2008), and the role of pFC in WM (Stokes, 2015) to name a few. (At this stage, the reader may have noted the involvement of Mark Stokes in several of these upheavals. It is not likely to be coincidence.)Throughout this journey over these last decades, TMS has been present, providing causal results to complement correlational neuroimaging, and behavioral studies. Despite its theoretical strengths as a class of research tools, the practical challenges and theoretical unknowns regarding physiological effects have meant that TMS has not always lived up to its promise as a “causal silver bullet” that can confirm correlational findings (Parkin, Ekhtiari, & Walsh, 2015; Siebner, Hartwigsen, Kassuba, & Rothwell, 2009).1 The future of TMS is, however, looking decidedly up (again). Advances in concurrent invasive recordings to visualize neuronal responses to TMS have provided invaluable in vivo evidence on the acute effects of stimulation (Allen, Pasley, Duong, & Freeman, 2007; Moliadze, Zhao, Eysel, & Funke, 2003). In humans, combining TMS with neuroimaging has also given the field a boost, showing effects across the brain (Bergmann et al., 2021), and on electrophysiological markers such as ERPs (Taylor, Walsh, & Eimer, 2008) and oscillations (Thut & Miniussi, 2009). Thus, as this commentary will argue, a crucial way forward for investigating cognition through neuroscience will be through innovative multimodal experimental approaches that can produce convergent evidence in one shot; examples will follow here as to how this approach can drive the field forward, with the focus being on TMS, given its superior spatial and temporal resolution compared with transcranial electrical stimulation.A game-changer for TMS has been in its simultaneous combination with fMRI and EEG. Although these innovations were demonstrated decades ago—for TMS-fMRI: Bohning et al. (1998); TMS-EEG: Ilmoniemi et al. (1997)—their progression from niche technical feats to essential contributors to cognitive neuroscience is coming to fruition. Given that one can now acquire reliable, well-supported systems for TMS-fMRI and TMS-EEG, the next step is underway in which advanced experimental and analysis approaches will answer questions that would otherwise be difficult to address.The first growth area of cognitive TMS research is in the combination of TMS-fMRI with functional and effective connectivity analysis approaches; TMS can be applied to intervene with brain networks necessary for behavior and causally establish the necessity of associated network nodes and communication pathways. Although there exist many sophisticated methods for establishing connectivity to uncover brain networks through advanced mathematical and statistical algorithms, they all provide indirect “best current estimates” (Johansen-Berg, 2013); what remains to be established is whether such connections exist beyond the statistical level and TMS is arguably the best placed to accomplish this. Nee and D'Esposito (2017) provided a valuable demonstration of what a TMS-fMRI-effective connectivity—here, dynamic causal modeling—approach can contribute theoretically. The use of TMS to causally test dynamic causal modelings has been suggested previously (Hartwigsen et al., 2015; Bestmann & Feredoes, 2013), and perturbation of a network with TMS has also been proposed in the context of network control theory (Medaglia, Pasqualetti, Hamilton, Thompson-Schill, & Bassett, 2017). A recent study by Sydnor et al. (2022) is a clear indication of the strengths of TMS-fMRI-perturbation, in which single pulses of TMS to ventrolateral pFC produced fMRI signal changes in the amygdala, the magnitude of which were predicted by the density of the white matter fiber pathway between the TMS target and amygdala. One could envisage a similar approach in which multiple lines of evidence including structure and function are combined to reveal patterns of connectivity for different types of behavioral tasks and/or different task stages. In the context of WM, such an approach may contribute to the ongoing debate on the nature of communication between sensory, parietal, and prefrontal cortex during WM maintenance (Gayet, Paffen, & Van der Stigchel, 2018; Scimeca, Kiyonaga, & D'Esposito, 2018; Xu, 2017), or the involvement of, and interactions between, more established networks such as the default mode and frontoparietal networks during WM (Murphy, Bertolero, Papadopoulos, Lydon-Staley, & Bassett, 2020).In addition to brain networks, the cognitive neuroscience field is interested in what information is being coded by brain regions engaged for a behavior. Here, decoding techniques such as multivoxel pattern analysis (MVPA) have proven to be useful (e.g., Woolgar, Jackson, & Duncan, 2016; Postle, 2015), and recently, they have been combined with fMRI (Jackson, Feredoes, Rich, Lindner, & Woolgar, 2021) and EEG (Rose et al., 2016) to provide answers to otherwise difficult-to-resolve questions, with the bonus of causal inference. Jackson et al. (2021) addressed a long-standing debate on relevant information representation across the brain in the face of irrelevant information. In this study, concurrent TMS-fMRI (i.e., TMS applied online during task performance in the MRI scanner) data were analyzed with MVPA, to show causally that the multiple demand network (Duncan, 2010) enhances relevant information rather than suppresses irrelevant information. Specifically, right dorsolateral pFC (DLPFC) TMS disrupted task performance, resulting in a decrease in the decoding of relevant information across the multiple demand network, whereas irrelevant information decoding was not significantly modulated by disruptive TMS. MVPA provided a unique way in which to view the effects of TMS, that is, on information coding, rather than the magnitude of the BOLD response and which in this case was critical for testing the impact of DLPFC on the same information being represented under different behavioral contexts.Rose et al. (2016) combined TMS with EEG (with fMRI informing the location of the cortical TMS target) to test a cognitive account of WM in which information can be held in different states depending on task demands (Oberauer, 2009). A latent, undecodable WM item (rendered thus because it was not required during a specific epoch of the task) could be “reactivated” with a single TMS “ping.”2 Specifically, the inactive item could be decoded for a brief period post-TMS, indicating that it had been brought into a different physiological state by TMS such that it could be detected with MVPA. This result marries very well with multiple-states theoretical accounts of WM (Stokes, 2015; Oberauer, 2009; Cowan, 2005), and demonstrates the unique ability of a TMS ping to access an otherwise inaccessible brain state.Combining TMS with neuroimaging has taken a further, dramatic step with simultaneous TMS-fMRI-EEG. While a technically extremely challenging feat, this approach is arguably a “holy grail” for some in the brain stimulation field, providing a bridge between the spatial and temporal, to capture oscillatory activity changes across the whole brain in response to perturbation with TMS. Proof of concept was demonstrated by Peters et al. (2013), with a cognitive application published more recently (Peters et al., 2020) in which right dorsal premotor cortex was stimulated with a short burst of below motor-threshold TMS and the state of pre-TMS alpha and low beta oscillations were examined to determine how trial-by-trial fluctuations influenced the effect of TMS throughout a functional motor network. TMS-induced signal propagation, as indexed by the fMRI response throughout the cortical–subcortical network of interest, was impeded by stronger pre-TMS alpha power, in line with alpha serving a suppressive function in terms of information transmission. The promise of this technique is in its ability to probe dynamic brain states, including those which exist during attention, memory, or cognitive control (Sack, 2022), and it is certainly in line with views such as those put forward by, for example, Mark Stokes, in which multiple brain areas combine while maintaining a highly dynamic state, to achieve flexible behavior (Stokes & Duncan, 2014). TMS-fMRI-EEG would very much be in the “watch this exciting space closely” category.Computational modeling of behavior can now also be added as an experimental approach benefitting from the addition of TMS. Again, relying on the perturbation logic, the spread of the consequences of TMS throughout hidden layers can provide a causal missing link, particularly if applied to existing TMS data (e.g., TMS-behavioral or TMS-neuroimaging data). Very recent work (Whyte, 2022) has taken this exact approach, applying the WM model of Manohar, Pertzov, and Husain (2017) to explain the behavioral and TMS effects on fMRI decoding of stimuli of Jackson et al. (2021). According to the model, stimuli and associated response rules are coded through rapid synaptic changes. Modeling a short TMS train to DLPFC as a brief excitatory input, the effect on the model was to produce a disruption of conjunction units coding task rules, assumed to be in the multiple demand network (Woolgar, Hampshire, Thompson, & Duncan, 2011). The results of the modeling could explain the fMRI decoding and behavioral results reported by Jackson et al. (2021). This plastic attractor model provides an account of DLPFC that permits adaptive, flexible behavior to select task-relevant stimulus information and to produce appropriate motor actions in response. The added value of TMS here was to provide a controlled perturbation of the network with behavioral and neural outcomes, with the computational model revealing the otherwise hidden underlying mechanisms. It is a fruitful area of future research, given that at some stage, computational models of brain and behavior will need causal verification in the same way as for mathematically derived functional brain networks, as described above.A final area that will be discussed here, for future cognitive TMS studies to pursue more ardently, is combining of patterned stimulation protocols with neuroimaging. There are a range of patterns in which TMS can be applied, producing complex physiological effects such as spike timing-dependent plasticity, to investigate cortico-cortico connectivity, for example, cortico-cortical paired associative stimulation (ccPAS; Romei, Thut, & Silvanto, 2016; Rizzo et al., 2009). Casula, Pellicciari, Picazio, Caltagirone, and Koch (2016) applied a ccPAS protocol to investigate the direction of communication between key nodes of the frontoparietal network, DLPFC, and posterior parietal cortex. Concurrent EEG (in the absence of behavior) showed an increase in high frontal gamma activity following DLPFC-parietal ccPAS, with the reverse when parietal cortex was stimulated first. The timing of these changes also fit with a GABA-dependent mechanism, suggesting a change in the amount of GABA available to DLPFC pyramidal neurons that generated the EEG signal (Casula et al., 2016). The interesting confluence of findings from this study can certainly motivate similar studies combining neuroimaging, and perhaps also neuropharmacology to generate unique research questions regarding the neural pathways and mechanisms underlying cognition (Pitcher, Parkin, & Walsh, 2021; Romei et al., 2016).The clear improvements in the field of combining TMS with neuroimaging and analysis, such as developments in hardware (Navarro de Lara et al., 2015), understanding the effects of TMS at the level of brain regions (Tik et al., 2019) and neuronally (Romero, Davare, Armendariz, & Janssen, 2019), and modeling of TMS effects (Shirinpour et al., 2021), all contribute to increasing the accuracy of TMS for causally perturbing brain and behavior. There are more sophisticated questions being asked by this field, producing more sophisticated evidence, compared with the “early days” of cognitive TMS. It is indeed an exciting time to be in the noninvasive neurostimulation field, with its potential to have a central role in informing cognitive neuroscientific theory growing with each publication.The author is supported by a Royal Society Leverhulme Trust Senior Research Fellowship.Reprint requests should be sent to Eva Feredoes, School of Psychology and Clinical Language Sciences, University of Reading, Earley Gate, Reading, RG2 7AG, UK, or via e-mail: e.a.feredoes@reading.ac.uk.Retrospective analysis of the citations in every article published in this journal from 2010 to 2021 reveals a persistent pattern of gender imbalance: Although the proportions of authorship teams (categorized by estimated gender identification of first author/last author) publishing in the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience (JoCN) during this period were M(an)/M = .407, W(oman)/M = .32, M/W = .115, and W/W = .159, the comparable proportions for the articles that these authorship teams cited were M/M = .549, W/M = .257, M/W = .109, and W/W = .085 (Postle and Fulvio, JoCN, 34:1, pp. 1–3). Consequently, JoCN encourages all authors to consider gender balance explicitly when selecting which articles to cite and gives them the opportunity to report their article’s gender citation balance. The authors of this article report its proportions of citations by gender category to be as follows: M/M = .615; W/M = .256; M/W = .077; W/W = .051.
最长约 10秒,即可获得该文献文件

科研通智能强力驱动
Strongly Powered by AbleSci AI
更新
PDF的下载单位、IP信息已删除 (2025-6-4)

科研通是完全免费的文献互助平台,具备全网最快的应助速度,最高的求助完成率。 对每一个文献求助,科研通都将尽心尽力,给求助人一个满意的交代。
实时播报
楚奇完成签到,获得积分10
刚刚
甜蜜的曼冬完成签到 ,获得积分10
1秒前
合适的平安完成签到 ,获得积分10
1秒前
新之助完成签到,获得积分10
2秒前
上官发布了新的文献求助10
2秒前
lqkcqmu发布了新的文献求助30
3秒前
呼噜完成签到,获得积分10
3秒前
研友_VZG7GZ应助燕子采纳,获得30
3秒前
高贵的乐天完成签到 ,获得积分10
3秒前
Hello应助阿瓦隆的蓝胖子采纳,获得30
3秒前
少年白777完成签到,获得积分10
3秒前
王金豪完成签到,获得积分10
3秒前
4秒前
Yellue发布了新的文献求助10
4秒前
drl完成签到,获得积分10
4秒前
CodeCraft应助xiaomili采纳,获得10
7秒前
7秒前
7秒前
爆米花应助啦啦啦采纳,获得10
8秒前
新之助发布了新的文献求助10
8秒前
8秒前
qw1完成签到,获得积分20
8秒前
舒适的尔容完成签到,获得积分10
8秒前
Ryan完成签到,获得积分10
9秒前
高大笙完成签到,获得积分10
9秒前
兑润泽完成签到,获得积分10
9秒前
9秒前
reece完成签到 ,获得积分10
9秒前
10秒前
wsh071117完成签到,获得积分10
10秒前
小蘑菇应助mwy采纳,获得10
10秒前
无花果应助刘莅采纳,获得10
10秒前
10秒前
ED应助lqkcqmu采纳,获得10
11秒前
11秒前
11秒前
高贵以珊发布了新的文献求助10
12秒前
12秒前
12秒前
明理的惜雪完成签到,获得积分10
13秒前
高分求助中
A new approach to the extrapolation of accelerated life test data 1000
‘Unruly’ Children: Historical Fieldnotes and Learning Morality in a Taiwan Village (New Departures in Anthropology) 400
Indomethacinのヒトにおける経皮吸収 400
Phylogenetic study of the order Polydesmida (Myriapoda: Diplopoda) 370
基于可调谐半导体激光吸收光谱技术泄漏气体检测系统的研究 330
Robot-supported joining of reinforcement textiles with one-sided sewing heads 320
Aktuelle Entwicklungen in der linguistischen Forschung 300
热门求助领域 (近24小时)
化学 材料科学 医学 生物 工程类 有机化学 生物化学 物理 内科学 纳米技术 计算机科学 化学工程 复合材料 遗传学 基因 物理化学 催化作用 冶金 细胞生物学 免疫学
热门帖子
关注 科研通微信公众号,转发送积分 3986829
求助须知:如何正确求助?哪些是违规求助? 3529292
关于积分的说明 11244137
捐赠科研通 3267685
什么是DOI,文献DOI怎么找? 1803843
邀请新用户注册赠送积分活动 881223
科研通“疑难数据库(出版商)”最低求助积分说明 808600