已入深夜,您辛苦了!由于当前在线用户较少,发布求助请尽量完整的填写文献信息,科研通机器人24小时在线,伴您度过漫漫科研夜!祝你早点完成任务,早点休息,好梦!

Antilocality and Antiagreement

语言学 哲学 历史
作者
Nico Baier
出处
期刊:Linguistic Inquiry [MIT Press]
卷期号:48 (2): 367-377 被引量:18
标识
DOI:10.1162/ling_a_00246
摘要

In many languages, the normal pattern of φ-agreement with an argument in a specific position (usually a subject) is disrupted when that argument is involved in an Ā-dependency. Since Ouhalla 1993, this phenomenon has been known as the antiagreement effect (henceforth antiagreement). Antiagreement is found in a wide variety of languages, but there is little consensus about the theoretical principles that underlie it.1 One prominent line of thought holds that antiagreement arises because of antilocality constraints on Ā-movement (Cheng 2006, Erlewine 2016, Schneider-Zioga 2007). The core intuition behind these accounts is that certain agreeing arguments are too local to Spec,CP to move there; this forces these arguments to extract from alternative positions, bleeding agreement.In this squib, I examine the relationship between antilocality and antiagreement, focusing on the version of antilocality developed by Erlewine (2016). Erlewine proposes a constraint that rules out Ā-movement from a specifier to the next highest specifier in the structure (the Spec-to-Spec Antilocality constraint; see (2)). I show that this constraint cannot derive antiagreement in Berber, which has become a canonical instance of the effect since Ouhalla's original article. This calls into question whether Spec-to-Spec Antilocality can be extended to a general account of antiagreement. I also show that these observations apply to other antilocality-based approaches to antiagreement in the literature (Cheng 2006, Schneider-Zioga 2007). This highlights a general property of theories of antilocality: namely, these theories are fragile, in that they are very sensitive to minor differences in clause structure, both within a single language and crosslinguistically.The remainder of this squib is structured as follows. In section 2, I introduce Erlewine's (2006) analysis of agent focus in the Mayan language Kaqchikel and his Spec-to-Spec Antilocality constraint. In section 3, I discuss antiagreement in Berber and necessary elements of Berber clause structure. In section 4, I show why Erlewine's analysis of antiagreement cannot be extended to Berber. In section 5, I discuss implications of these observations.Erlewine (2016) examines the distribution of the so-called agent focus (AF) form in the Mayan language Kaqchikel, shown in (1).2Example (1a) is a canonical transitive clause in Kaqchikel. The verb agrees with both arguments in an ergative-absolutive pattern. In (1b), the transitive subject has undergone Ā-movement to Spec,CP. There is no ergative agreement; instead, the verb takes an AF suffix. Ergative agreement is ungrammatical in such contexts. AF is limited to extraction of transitive subjects; Ā-movement of other arguments does not trigger the AF form.Erlewine pursues the idea that AF arises because of a constraint on Ā-movement that differentially affects transitive subjects and other arguments. He proposes the following constraint:This constraint rules out movement of the type in (3), but allows movement of the type in (4).Erlewine argues that the φ-probe responsible for ergative agreement in Kaqchikel is located on T and that it has the EPP property. Thus, ergative agreement requires movement of the external argument to Spec,TP.3 Agreeing transitive subjects are therefore too local to Spec,CP to undergo Ā-movement, as shown in (5).Instead, transitive subjects must extract from their base position in Spec,vP; they skip Spec,TP altogether, moving directly to Spec,CP, as in (6).Because ergative agreement is parasitic on the EPP, there can be no ergative agreement. The AF form is a morphological repair in this context.4 Crucial evidence for this account comes from the fact that material intervening between the extracted transitive subject and the verb obviates the need for AF.5Under the assumption that preverbal adverbs are hosted by a dedicated phrasal projection (Cinque 1999), extraction in (7) crosses sufficient structure and therefore does not violate (2).6Erlewine classifies the Kaqchikel AF construction as a case of antiagreement and argues that SSAL can be extended to a more general account of antiagreement crosslinguistically. He shows that his analysis can account for the distribution of antiagreement in the Tupian language Karitiâna (Storto 1999) and the northern Italian dialects Trentino and Fiorentino. Brandi and Cordin (1989) show that in these dialects, postverbal subjects require default agreement. Ā-movement of the subject requires the same paradigm as postverbal subjects, even though the subject precedes the verb. Consider the Fiorentino data in (9).Erlewine argues that this is also attributable to SSAL. A fully agreeing preverbal subject would be too close to Spec,CP to undergo Ā-movement. Instead, extraction takes place from a low, postverbal position. As in Kaqchikel, movement skips Spec,TP entirely, bleeding the normal agreement that one sees when the subject lands there.Erlewine's account is based on the assumption that languages exhibiting antiagreement share the crucial structural property in (10).This makes a strong prediction: if an argument is able to control agreement on X without being located at β in (10), then that argument should be able to undergo Ā-movement without the loss of agreement. Extraction from a position below β should be uninhibited by antilocality, and therefore, agreement should not be affected. In the next section, I show that subject agreement in Berber is not parasitic on movement to such a position.Since Ouhalla's (1993) original article on antiagreement, Berber has been cited as a canonical example of the phenomenon. Consider the Tarifit Berber data in (11).Example (11a) is a declarative clause containing a postverbal subject, the ordinary position of subjects in Berber. The verb agrees with the subject for person, gender, and number. In (11b), the subject has undergone Ā-movement to Spec,CP. In this context, normal subject-verb agreement is impossible; instead, the verb is in a form called the "participle" in the Berber literature. The participle is invariant with regard to the φ-features of the subject; it does not reflect the φ-features of the subject in any way.This effect is also found in subject relative clauses and subject focus constructions, given in (12a) and (12b), respectively.Object Ā-movement does not trigger antiagreement. In (13), only full agreement is possible.Thus, like AF in Kaqchikel, antiagreement in Berber is characteristic of certain Ā-moved arguments, in this case subjects.7 Ā-movement of other arguments (and Ā-movement of adjuncts) does not trigger antiagreement.8In order to examine Erlewine's account of antiagreement in relation to Berber, it is necessary to establish (a) where the φ-probe responsible for agreement is located in the clausal spine and (b) where agreeing subjects are located in the clause. In this section, I present evidence that agreement features are located below TP and that subjects value these features from their base position inside vP.There is a general consensus in the Berber literature that VSO word order is derived by verb movement to a presubject position (Omari 2001, Ouali 2011, Ouhalla 1988, 2005). However, there is disagreement on exactly how far the verb raises. In his work on Tamazight Berber, Ouali (2011) argues for the clause structure in (14).Ouali argues that the verb can raise to either Asp or T, but that it always moves at least to Asp, as Tamazight verbs are always inflected for aspect. Whether or not the verb raises to T is determined by the nature of that head. Tamazight (like other Berber languages) has a set of uninflected tense particles. When there is an overt tense particle, the verb remains in Asp, as in (15). When there is no overt tense particle, the verb raises to T, as in (16).Evidence for this analysis comes from the position of the verb in relation to object clitics. When there is an overt particle in T, the verb follows object clitics, as in (17a). When there is no overt particle, the verb precedes the clitics, as in (17b).This variation is explained under the assumption that clitics are located between TP and AspP.Note that the verb bears subject agreement in each of (15)–(17) regardless of whether it raises to Asp or to T. I take this as evidence that the φ-probe responsible for subject-verb agreement is located in a head below T, the position commonly assumed to host the subject φ-probe. This explains why the verb still bears agreement when it has only moved to Asp. In what follows, I will assume that the φ-probe is located on Asp in Berber.It is also generally assumed that the subject triggers agreement from its thematic position in Berber; subjects need not raise to trigger agreement. Evidence for this comes from the placement of manner adverbs, which can occur between V and S. This is shown in (18), where the manner adverb deghya 'quickly' comes between the verb and the postverbal subject.Assuming that such adverbs merge above vP, then example (18) provides evidence that the verb has moved out of vP while the subject remains low inside vP.It is also possible that there is a separate, dedicated head in the region of AspP that hosts φ-features (call it Agr), perhaps between Asp and v. Michael Erlewine (pers. comm.) points out that in this scenario, the SSAL account could be extended to Berber given two further assumptions: (a) Agr agrees in a specifier-head configuration and (b) AspP is a phase edge. This alternative makes the strong prediction that manner adverbs like deghya in (18) should ameliorate antiagreement, under the assumption that such adverbs are hosted in the specifier of a functional projection. In fact, this prediction is not borne out.Subject extraction in (19) requires antiagreement even though a manner adverb is present. This is exactly the opposite of what we expect under a SSAL account of antiagreement tied to the lower phase-edge.9We are left with a picture of Berber clause structure where agreement occurs in a low region of the clause. There are φ-features on Asp that probe and find the subject in Spec,vP. This is schematized in (20).In the next section, I show why these data are problematic for Erlewine's (2016) account of antiagreement.Recall that under Erlewine's (2016) SSAL analysis, movement of a subject from Spec,TP is blocked in languages like Kaqchikel and Fiorentino. Instead, they extract from lower in the structure.The derivation in (22) bleeds agreement with the subject on the assumption that agreement is dependent on EPP movement to Spec,TP. It is forced by Erlewine's SSAL constraint, which blocks Ā-movement from one specifier to the next higher specifier (see (2)).We concluded in the previous section that subjects in Berber stay low, inside vP. Agreement features probe the subject in situ and are also located in a low position in the clause. Given these conclusions, there should be no problem in extracting subjects from their base position in Spec,vP to Spec,CP. Ā-movement of the subject crosses at least TP and AspP.The Ā-movement path taken by the subject in (23) does not violate SSAL, and therefore antiagreement in Berber does not receive an explanation under Erlewine's theory.10 Specifically, the Berber data provide an argument against antilocality approaches to antiagreement in which φ-agreement is dependent on A-movement into a specifier directly below the projection targeted by Ā-movement.11The narrow implication of this discussion is that Erlewine's (2016) analysis of antiagreement in Kaqchikel, northern Italian dialects, and Karitiâna in terms of SSAL cannot be easily extended to cover the distribution of antiagreement in Berber, which has been taken as a canonical instance of the effect since Ouhalla's (1993) original article. While this is a very specific conclusion, I believe it is important, as Erlewine suggests that SSAL can offer a general account of antiagreement crosslinguistically.Moreover, the Berber data present a more general problem for any antilocality theory of antiagreement. These accounts are based on the intuition that Ā-movement cannot proceed from a position normally targeted for agreement, because that position is "too close" to the landing site of Ā-movement. Antilocality forces movement from an alternative position, thereby bleeding agreement. However, in Berber, subjects agree from their base-generated position; there is no alternative non-agreement-triggering position. Thus, the basic logic of the antilocality account cannot be applied to Berber.As a more concrete example, consider how these data bear on Grohmann's (2003) theory of antilocality, which has been used to derive antiagreement in Bantu by Cheng (2006) and Schneider-Zioga (2007). Under Grohmann's theory, clauses are standardly separated into three domains: the theta domain (θΔ; vP/VP and its articulation), the agreement domain (ΦΔ; TP and its articulation), and the discourse domain (ΩΔ; CP and its articulation). Movement within the same domain is banned; that is, such movement is "too short." Assuming that the subject does not raise above Spec,vP to agree in Berber, a domain-based antilocality account faces a challenge similar to the one facing the Spec-to-Spec movement approach. Ā-movement from Spec,vP to Spec,CP should never be too short (in this case within the same domain). This is because such movement will always involve cross-domain movement from the theta domain into the discourse domain, as shown in (24).More generally, these observations highlight a general property of theories of antilocality: namely, such accounts are fragile, in that they are very sensitive to minor differences in clause structure, both within a single language and crosslinguistically. For SSAL, this fragility arises from the very specific configuration required to incur a violation that would lead to antiagreement (see (10)). The same basic problem extends to domain-based antilocality. While domains may be more robust in terms of the range of clause structures they can apply to, the configuration needed to violate the constraint remains quite sensitive to variation.The structural differences between Berber on the one hand and Kaqchikel and the northern Italian dialects on the other cast doubt on whether there is a single shared structural property that sets languages that exhibit antiagreement apart from those that do not. Any potentially general theory of antiagreement must contend with these differences. Therefore, a broader implication of this squib is that antiagreement may not be a unified phenomenon, as the literature has implicitly treated it until now. Instead, we may be dealing with a set of discrete, separate effects that conspire to create the illusion of a single, unified phenomenon.

科研通智能强力驱动
Strongly Powered by AbleSci AI
更新
大幅提高文件上传限制,最高150M (2024-4-1)

科研通是完全免费的文献互助平台,具备全网最快的应助速度,最高的求助完成率。 对每一个文献求助,科研通都将尽心尽力,给求助人一个满意的交代。
实时播报
1秒前
华仔应助神介.Tzx采纳,获得10
1秒前
liuheqian完成签到,获得积分10
2秒前
8秒前
9秒前
10秒前
Louis完成签到,获得积分10
11秒前
大云发布了新的文献求助10
15秒前
大模型应助Louis采纳,获得10
16秒前
18秒前
小武完成签到,获得积分10
18秒前
纯洁发布了新的文献求助10
19秒前
可可完成签到 ,获得积分10
20秒前
21秒前
酷波er应助大云采纳,获得10
23秒前
神介.Tzx发布了新的文献求助10
24秒前
执着瓜6发布了新的文献求助10
26秒前
态度完成签到,获得积分10
31秒前
斯文败类应助温暖静柏采纳,获得10
36秒前
招水若离完成签到,获得积分10
36秒前
朴实的秋完成签到,获得积分10
37秒前
吱吱吱完成签到 ,获得积分10
38秒前
39秒前
纯洁完成签到,获得积分10
39秒前
执着瓜6完成签到,获得积分10
39秒前
43秒前
43秒前
烂漫的绝悟完成签到 ,获得积分10
43秒前
krkr完成签到,获得积分10
45秒前
youngyang完成签到 ,获得积分10
45秒前
温暖静柏完成签到,获得积分20
45秒前
罗浩禹发布了新的文献求助10
48秒前
52秒前
52秒前
玩命的毛衣完成签到 ,获得积分10
54秒前
57秒前
大模型应助大砍刀采纳,获得10
58秒前
温暖静柏发布了新的文献求助10
58秒前
星辰大海应助眼睛大香芦采纳,获得10
1分钟前
1分钟前
高分求助中
Licensing Deals in Pharmaceuticals 2019-2024 3000
Cognitive Paradigms in Knowledge Organisation 2000
Introduction to Spectroscopic Ellipsometry of Thin Film Materials Instrumentation, Data Analysis, and Applications 1800
Natural History of Mantodea 螳螂的自然史 1000
A Photographic Guide to Mantis of China 常见螳螂野外识别手册 800
How Maoism Was Made: Reconstructing China, 1949-1965 800
Barge Mooring (Oilfield Seamanship Series Volume 6) 600
热门求助领域 (近24小时)
化学 医学 生物 材料科学 工程类 有机化学 生物化学 物理 内科学 纳米技术 计算机科学 化学工程 复合材料 基因 遗传学 催化作用 物理化学 免疫学 量子力学 细胞生物学
热门帖子
关注 科研通微信公众号,转发送积分 3314323
求助须知:如何正确求助?哪些是违规求助? 2946571
关于积分的说明 8530887
捐赠科研通 2622334
什么是DOI,文献DOI怎么找? 1434442
科研通“疑难数据库(出版商)”最低求助积分说明 665310
邀请新用户注册赠送积分活动 650855