亲爱的研友该休息了!由于当前在线用户较少,发布求助请尽量完整的填写文献信息,科研通机器人24小时在线,伴您度过漫漫科研夜!身体可是革命的本钱,早点休息,好梦!

Memory Control: A Fundamental Mechanism of Emotion Regulation

心理学 机制(生物学) 控制(管理) 认知科学 认知心理学 神经科学 认识论 人工智能 计算机科学 哲学
作者
Haakon Engen,Michael C. Anderson
出处
期刊:Trends in Cognitive Sciences [Elsevier]
卷期号:22 (11): 982-995 被引量:146
标识
DOI:10.1016/j.tics.2018.07.015
摘要

The neural correlates of emotion regulation have been studied extensively over the past decade, but without consideration of the mechanistic role of memory control. Knowledge of the neurocognitive mechanisms enabling the suppression of memories and thoughts from conscious awareness has grown increasingly specific. Recent studies reveal an association between memory control deficits and affective psychopathologies such as depression and post-traumatic stress disorder, suggesting that controlling memory and affect are interrelated. Successfully controlling the retrieval of intrusive memories reduces their later emotional impact via modulation of amygdala activity, indicating that controlling memories regulates affect. Memory control processes may provide a mechanistic foundation for emotion regulation, thereby contributing to a unified account of the mechanisms underlying this process. Memories play a ubiquitous role in our emotional lives, both causing vivid emotional experiences in their own right and imbuing perception of the external world with emotional significance. Controlling the emotional impact of memories therefore poses a major emotion-regulation challenge, suggesting that there might be a hitherto unexplored link between the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying memory control (MC) and emotion regulation. We present here a theoretical account of how the mechanisms of MC constitute core component processes of cognitive emotion regulation (CER), and how this observation may help to understand its basic mechanisms and their disruption in psychiatric disorders. Memories play a ubiquitous role in our emotional lives, both causing vivid emotional experiences in their own right and imbuing perception of the external world with emotional significance. Controlling the emotional impact of memories therefore poses a major emotion-regulation challenge, suggesting that there might be a hitherto unexplored link between the neurocognitive mechanisms underlying memory control (MC) and emotion regulation. We present here a theoretical account of how the mechanisms of MC constitute core component processes of cognitive emotion regulation (CER), and how this observation may help to understand its basic mechanisms and their disruption in psychiatric disorders. Affective science has often sought to explain how external stimuli cause emotional reactions [1Gross J.J. Barrett L.F. Emotion generation and emotion regulation: one or two depends on your point of view.Emot. Rev. 2011; 3: 8-16Crossref PubMed Scopus (193) Google Scholar]. Following from this, research on emotion regulation has emphasized (with notable exceptions [2Kross E. et al.Coping with emotions past: the neural bases of regulating affect associated with negative autobiographical memories.Biol. Psychiatry. 2009; 65: 361-366Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (138) Google Scholar, 3Kross E. et al.When asking 'why' does not hurt. Distinguishing rumination from reflective processing of negative emotions.Psychol. Sci. 2005; 16: 709-715Crossref PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar, 4Cooney R.E. et al.Neural correlates of rumination in depression.Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2010; 10: 470-478Crossref PubMed Scopus (158) Google Scholar]) the mechanisms that enable us to pursue our goals in the face of external emotional stressors [5Gross J.J. The extended process model of emotion regulation: elaborations, applications, and future directions.Psychol. Inq. 2015; 26: 130-137Crossref Scopus (31) Google Scholar]. Understanding how people regulate exogenously elicited emotions (see Glossary) is, and should be, a fundamental goal of emotion regulation research. However, research on emotion regulation would benefit from a mechanistic understanding of an equally great – perhaps even greater – source of emotional states in our daily lives: memory. Outside the immediate experience of emotional events, any later impact – adaptive or maladaptive – that they have on our lives is mediated by memory. Like Proust's madeleines, the triggers for this are often stimuli that most people would consider innocuous, but that by virtue of evoking an episodic memory in the individual have the power to elicit neural, physiological, and subjective reactions mimicking those of the original experience [6Daselaar S.M. et al.The spatiotemporal dynamics of autobiographical memory: neural correlates of recall, emotional intensity, and reliving.Cereb. Cortex. 2008; 18: 217-229Crossref PubMed Scopus (171) Google Scholar, 7Engen H.G. et al.The neural component-process architecture of endogenously generated emotion.Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017; 12: 197-211PubMed Google Scholar]. Thus, people can ruminate on past emotionally significant events, frequently leading to the reinstatement and perpetuation of (mostly negative) emotions from their past. Similarly, when people worry about their futures, they engage episodic prospection processes that can engender endogenous emotional reactions even though the eliciting events have not occurred [8Schacter D.L. et al.Episodic simulation of future events.Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008; 1124: 39-60Crossref PubMed Scopus (418) Google Scholar]. Such self-generated emotional states can be a major source of stress and are thought to play a central role in the etiology of psychopathology [9Morina N. et al.Prospective mental imagery in patients with major depressive disorder or anxiety disorders.J. Anxiety Disord. 2011; 25: 1032-1037Crossref PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar, 10Brosschot J.F. et al.The perseverative cognition hypothesis: a review of worry, prolonged stress-related physiological activation, and health.J. Psychosom. Res. 2006; 60: 113-124Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (619) Google Scholar]. Indeed, this is likely the case even for disorders primarily characterized by aberrant reactions to stimuli, because even exogenously elicited emotions are robustly influenced by memory: emotional experiences are not mere reactions to the environment but are caused by both the properties of the stimulus and the context in which it occurs [11Barrett L.F. Constructing emotion.Psychol. Top. 2011; 20: 359-380Google Scholar, 12Lindquist K.A. Barrett L.F. Constructing emotion: the experience of fear as a conceptual act.Psychol. Sci. 2008; 19: 898-903Crossref PubMed Scopus (81) Google Scholar, 13Barrett L.F. The theory of constructed emotion: an active inference account of interoception and categorization.Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2017; 12: 1-23Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 14Sander D. et al.A systems approach to appraisal mechanisms in emotion.Neural Netw. 2005; 18: 317-352Crossref PubMed Scopus (282) Google Scholar, 15Scherer K.R. The dynamic architecture of emotion: evidence for the component process model.Cogn. Emot. 2009; 23: 1307-1351Crossref Google Scholar]. This context consists not only of physical factors (e.g., whether one encounters a snake in a terrarium or on a forest path) but also one's history with a stimulus and the expectations thus engendered; expectations that are rooted in internal predictive models [16Sterling P. Allostasis: a model of predictive regulation.Physiol. Behav. 2012; 106: 5-15Crossref PubMed Scopus (142) Google Scholar, 17Chanes L. Barrett L.F. Redefining the role of limbic areas in cortical processing.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016; 20: 96-106Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (46) Google Scholar], which are themselves grounded in memories [18Hasson U. et al.Hierarchical process memory: memory as an integral component of information processing.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015; 19: 304-313Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Google Scholar]. Consequently, if it were possible to modulate the accessibility of memories, one could change either exogenously or endogenously generated emotional responses – in effect, regulating emotion [1Gross J.J. Barrett L.F. Emotion generation and emotion regulation: one or two depends on your point of view.Emot. Rev. 2011; 3: 8-16Crossref PubMed Scopus (193) Google Scholar, 19Gross J.J. et al.Emotion generation and emotion regulation: a distinction we should make (carefully).Cogn. Emot. 2011; 25: 765-781Crossref PubMed Scopus (100) Google Scholar]. Despite these illustrations of the important role of memories in shaping our emotional lives, how we regulate emotions by downregulating the memory traces that contribute to them has attracted comparatively little attention in research on emotion regulation. In this Opinion article we outline a view of the mechanisms supporting the regulation of mnemonic emotional material building on emerging findings from research on memory control (MC). Our starting point is the observation that, because our emotions are frequently driven by memories, controlling the accessibility of such memories to awareness should be an effective way of regulating one's emotional reactions [20Anderson M.C. Hanslmayr S. Neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014; 18: 279-292Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Google Scholar]. Nevertheless, various factors have contributed to the view that attempts to voluntarily suppress thoughts and memories are unhelpful, and are perhaps harmful (Box 1). Contrary to this, we claim that controlling the occurrence and/or constitution of an upsetting memory – in short, MC – is the ultimate objective of successful emotion regulation of endogenous materials.Box 1Differentiating Direct Retrieval Suppression from Other Suppressive PhenomenaA potential source of confusion about our argument is the tendency to equate DS with other, often counterproductive, suppressive phenomena. For example, in emotion regulation research 'suppression' usually refers to expressive suppression (ES), which involves inhibiting the behavioral expressions associated with emotional states (e.g., adopting a poker face to hide emotions). Owing to its small or even paradoxical effects on subjective emotion [74Gross J.J. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology.J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1998; 74: 224-237Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 85Goldin P.R. et al.The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion.Biol. Psychiatry. 2008; 63: 577-586Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (785) Google Scholar], and because the trait tendency to use ES is associated with poor mental health outcomes [86John O.P. Gross J.J. Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: personality processes, individual differences, and life span development.J. Pers. 2004; 72: 1301-1333Crossref PubMed Scopus (759) Google Scholar], ES is often considered to be a maladaptive emotion-regulation strategy. However, DS clearly differs from ES in that it directly acts on the representation of the unwanted memory, rendering it less accessible. As such, DS is a fully cognitive strategy, involving different regulatory mechanisms.Another related line of research is thought suppression, as explored through the 'white bear effect'. In this research participants are instructed to not think of a specific item (e.g., a white bear) over a period of 5 minutes, and report when it comes to mind [87Wegner D.M. Schneider D.J. The white bear story.Psychol. Inq. 2003; 14: 326-329Crossref Google Scholar]. Research using this paradigm has shown that participants have difficulty avoiding thinking about the item, and this has been taken to show the inefficacy and counterproductive nature of thought suppression [87Wegner D.M. Schneider D.J. The white bear story.Psychol. Inq. 2003; 14: 326-329Crossref Google Scholar, 88Wegner D.M. et al.Paradoxical effects of thought suppression.J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1987; 53: 5-13Crossref PubMed Google Scholar]. While DS conceptually resembles the 'white bear' variety of thought suppression, the experimental procedure used to investigate the latter has characteristics that make suppression inherently unlikely to succeed. Specifically, the white bear task makes explicit reference to a specific forbidden thought to be suppressed. However, achieving this stated goal is effectively impossible because simply remembering the purpose of the task requires that participants violate the task goal. This contrasts with the DS task, which does not integrate the specific memory/thought to be avoided as part of the task goal, rendering retrieval suppression possible. This 'goal-integration theory' may account for the apparent discrepancy between work on MC and the Wegner thought-suppression paradigm [89Anderson M.C. Huddleston E. Towards a cognitive and neurobiological model of motivated forgetting.in: Belli R.F. True and False Recovered Memories. Springer, New York, NY2012: 53-120Crossref Scopus (73) Google Scholar], and suggests that the white bear task might fail to capture the true utility of suppressive processes measured in work on MC. Consistent with this, a meta-analysis [90Magee J.C. et al.Psychopathology and thought suppression: a quantitative review.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2012; 32: 189-201Crossref PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar] of 33 studies of psychopathology using the white bear task found no differences in suppression success or in rebound effects across patients and controls, raising concerns about whether this the task captures the mechanistic deficits that lead to intrusions in real life.Researchers have also investigated the relationship between thought suppression and psychopathology via the white bear suppression inventory (WBSI). Such work has contributed to the widely repeated conclusion that suppression, as a coping strategy, is associated with increased risk of psychopathology [91Aldao A. et al.Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010; 30: 217-237Crossref PubMed Scopus (1405) Google Scholar]. Although the WBSI was intended to measure tendency to suppress thoughts, large-scale studies have now repeatedly found that the scale measures both the tendency to suppress thoughts and the degree to which they fail at doing so [92Rassin E. The white bear suppression inventory (WBSI) focuses on failing suppression attempts.Eur. J. Pers. 2003; 17: 285-298Crossref Scopus (48) Google Scholar, 93Höping W. de Jong-Meyer R. Differentiating unwanted intrusive thoughts from thought suppression: what does the white bear suppression inventory measure?.Pers. Individ. Dif. 2003; 34: 1049-1055Crossref Scopus (0) Google Scholar, 94Blumberg S.J. The white bear suppression inventory: revisiting its factor structure.Pers. Individ. Dif. 2000; 29: 943-950Crossref Google Scholar]. Importantly, failure at, but not tendency to, suppress thoughts predicts psychopathology. Indeed, when successful thought-control ability is separately quantified [41Catarino A. et al.Failing to forget: inhibitory-control deficits compromise memory suppression in posttraumatic stress disorder.Psychol. Sci. 2015; 26: 604-616Crossref PubMed Scopus (34) Google Scholar] it robustly predicts reduced symptoms of anxiety, depression, and other conditions characterized by intrusive symptomatology. These findings are consistent with the possibility that, in normative samples, thought suppression may play an important beneficial role in mental health. A potential source of confusion about our argument is the tendency to equate DS with other, often counterproductive, suppressive phenomena. For example, in emotion regulation research 'suppression' usually refers to expressive suppression (ES), which involves inhibiting the behavioral expressions associated with emotional states (e.g., adopting a poker face to hide emotions). Owing to its small or even paradoxical effects on subjective emotion [74Gross J.J. Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology.J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1998; 74: 224-237Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 85Goldin P.R. et al.The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion.Biol. Psychiatry. 2008; 63: 577-586Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (785) Google Scholar], and because the trait tendency to use ES is associated with poor mental health outcomes [86John O.P. Gross J.J. Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: personality processes, individual differences, and life span development.J. Pers. 2004; 72: 1301-1333Crossref PubMed Scopus (759) Google Scholar], ES is often considered to be a maladaptive emotion-regulation strategy. However, DS clearly differs from ES in that it directly acts on the representation of the unwanted memory, rendering it less accessible. As such, DS is a fully cognitive strategy, involving different regulatory mechanisms. Another related line of research is thought suppression, as explored through the 'white bear effect'. In this research participants are instructed to not think of a specific item (e.g., a white bear) over a period of 5 minutes, and report when it comes to mind [87Wegner D.M. Schneider D.J. The white bear story.Psychol. Inq. 2003; 14: 326-329Crossref Google Scholar]. Research using this paradigm has shown that participants have difficulty avoiding thinking about the item, and this has been taken to show the inefficacy and counterproductive nature of thought suppression [87Wegner D.M. Schneider D.J. The white bear story.Psychol. Inq. 2003; 14: 326-329Crossref Google Scholar, 88Wegner D.M. et al.Paradoxical effects of thought suppression.J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1987; 53: 5-13Crossref PubMed Google Scholar]. While DS conceptually resembles the 'white bear' variety of thought suppression, the experimental procedure used to investigate the latter has characteristics that make suppression inherently unlikely to succeed. Specifically, the white bear task makes explicit reference to a specific forbidden thought to be suppressed. However, achieving this stated goal is effectively impossible because simply remembering the purpose of the task requires that participants violate the task goal. This contrasts with the DS task, which does not integrate the specific memory/thought to be avoided as part of the task goal, rendering retrieval suppression possible. This 'goal-integration theory' may account for the apparent discrepancy between work on MC and the Wegner thought-suppression paradigm [89Anderson M.C. Huddleston E. Towards a cognitive and neurobiological model of motivated forgetting.in: Belli R.F. True and False Recovered Memories. Springer, New York, NY2012: 53-120Crossref Scopus (73) Google Scholar], and suggests that the white bear task might fail to capture the true utility of suppressive processes measured in work on MC. Consistent with this, a meta-analysis [90Magee J.C. et al.Psychopathology and thought suppression: a quantitative review.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2012; 32: 189-201Crossref PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar] of 33 studies of psychopathology using the white bear task found no differences in suppression success or in rebound effects across patients and controls, raising concerns about whether this the task captures the mechanistic deficits that lead to intrusions in real life. Researchers have also investigated the relationship between thought suppression and psychopathology via the white bear suppression inventory (WBSI). Such work has contributed to the widely repeated conclusion that suppression, as a coping strategy, is associated with increased risk of psychopathology [91Aldao A. et al.Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review.Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010; 30: 217-237Crossref PubMed Scopus (1405) Google Scholar]. Although the WBSI was intended to measure tendency to suppress thoughts, large-scale studies have now repeatedly found that the scale measures both the tendency to suppress thoughts and the degree to which they fail at doing so [92Rassin E. The white bear suppression inventory (WBSI) focuses on failing suppression attempts.Eur. J. Pers. 2003; 17: 285-298Crossref Scopus (48) Google Scholar, 93Höping W. de Jong-Meyer R. Differentiating unwanted intrusive thoughts from thought suppression: what does the white bear suppression inventory measure?.Pers. Individ. Dif. 2003; 34: 1049-1055Crossref Scopus (0) Google Scholar, 94Blumberg S.J. The white bear suppression inventory: revisiting its factor structure.Pers. Individ. Dif. 2000; 29: 943-950Crossref Google Scholar]. Importantly, failure at, but not tendency to, suppress thoughts predicts psychopathology. Indeed, when successful thought-control ability is separately quantified [41Catarino A. et al.Failing to forget: inhibitory-control deficits compromise memory suppression in posttraumatic stress disorder.Psychol. Sci. 2015; 26: 604-616Crossref PubMed Scopus (34) Google Scholar] it robustly predicts reduced symptoms of anxiety, depression, and other conditions characterized by intrusive symptomatology. These findings are consistent with the possibility that, in normative samples, thought suppression may play an important beneficial role in mental health. More controversially perhaps, we further propose that MC mechanisms underlie the benefits of many of the volitional cognitive emotion regulation (CER) phenomena that have been identified, even when regulation is targeted at exogenously driven emotional reactions. Specifically, we claim that CER strategies can be classified by whether they employ two core mechanisms of MC that reduce access to memory traces that contribute to emotional responses. Importantly, we propose that the mechanisms of MC offer a novel account of how emotion regulation is implemented, that may help to understand its normal functioning and also psychopathological syndromes. This proposal is motivated by growing evidence that instructed MC abilities are related to beneficial outcomes (reviewed below) and the belief that similar mechanisms may support spontaneous MC, and therefore spontaneous CER, although further research will be necessary to test this assumption. As such, we propose that MC is crucial to regulating both mnemonically and exogenously elicited emotional states, and should be considered to be a fundamental process of volitional emotion regulation. MC can be defined as the capacity to volitionally influence the contents of thought in a goal-directed fashion by reducing the accessibility of memories [20Anderson M.C. Hanslmayr S. Neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014; 18: 279-292Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Google Scholar]. MC is often exerted reactively, in response to reminders that trigger the automatic retrieval of an unwanted memory or thought. Two mechanisms have been identified that enable MC when someone confronts an unwelcome reminder: (i) direct suppression (DS), involving the stopping or cancellation of the episodic retrieval process initiated by the cue, and the inhibition of the unwanted trace, and (ii) thought substitution (TS), involving the engagement of episodic retrieval, but instead being redirected towards alternative memories to occupy the limited focus of awareness, and to inhibit the to-be-avoided memory [20Anderson M.C. Hanslmayr S. Neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014; 18: 279-292Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Google Scholar]. Both of these mechanisms can be voluntarily deployed, and both induce forgetting of unwanted memories [21Küpper C.S. et al.Direct suppression as a mechanism for controlling unpleasant memories in daily life.J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2014; 143: 1443-1449Crossref PubMed Scopus (30) Google Scholar, 22Depue B.E. et al.Prefrontal regions orchestrate suppression of emotional memories via a two-phase process.Science. 2007; 317: 215-219Crossref PubMed Scopus (257) Google Scholar, 23Marzi T. et al.Emotions shape memory suppression in trait anxiety.Front. Psychol. 2014; 4: 1001Crossref PubMed Scopus (17) Google Scholar, 24van Schie K. et al.Emotional and non-emotional memories are suppressible under direct suppression instructions.Cogn. Emot. 2013; 27: 1122-1131Crossref PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar], enabling motivated forgetting of both neutral and affectively charged memories [20Anderson M.C. Hanslmayr S. Neural mechanisms of motivated forgetting.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2014; 18: 279-292Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Google Scholar, 24van Schie K. et al.Emotional and non-emotional memories are suppressible under direct suppression instructions.Cogn. Emot. 2013; 27: 1122-1131Crossref PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar, 25Benoit R.G. Anderson M.C. Opposing mechanisms support the voluntary forgetting of unwanted memories.Neuron. 2012; 76: 450-460Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (98) Google Scholar, 26Gagnepain P. et al.Parallel regulation of memory and emotion supports the suppression of intrusive memories.J. Neurosci. 2017; 37: 6423-6441Crossref PubMed Scopus (6) Google Scholar]. Crucially, because these processes allow a person to avoid awareness of memories, they are sometimes mistakenly equated with other forms of cognitive avoidance that are often associated with poor psychological outcomes [27Naragon-Gainey K. et al.The structure of common emotion regulation strategies: a meta-analytic examination.Psychol. Bull. 2017; 143: 384-427Crossref PubMed Scopus (9) Google Scholar]. The capacity to induce forgetting sets these mechanisms apart from conventional cognitive or behavioral avoidance: MC involves not simply avoiding reminders that trigger unwanted thoughts or memories but instead involves directly confronting those reminders and actively altering our cognitive response to them, and reducing the accessibility of the associated memory trace. DS of retrieval should not be confused with other forms of suppression that are often discussed in research on emotion regulation, and which have been argued to be ineffective and maladaptive (Box 1). Extensive evidence shows that suppressing retrieval in response to reminders is possible, and that doing so has persisting effects on the accessibility of suppressed traces [21Küpper C.S. et al.Direct suppression as a mechanism for controlling unpleasant memories in daily life.J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2014; 143: 1443-1449Crossref PubMed Scopus (30) Google Scholar, 25Benoit R.G. Anderson M.C. Opposing mechanisms support the voluntary forgetting of unwanted memories.Neuron. 2012; 76: 450-460Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (98) Google Scholar, 28Levy B. Inhibitory processes and the control of memory retrieval.Trends Cogn. Sci. 2002; 6: 299-305Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar, 29Levy B.J. Anderson M.C. Individual differences in the suppression of unwanted memories: the executive deficit hypothesis.Acta Psychol. 2008; 127: 623-635Crossref PubMed Scopus (126) Google Scholar, 30Anderson M.C. et al.Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories.Science. 2004; 303: 232-235Crossref PubMed Scopus (583) Google Scholar, 31Anderson M.C. Green C. Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control.Nature. 2001; 410: 366-369Crossref PubMed Scopus (512) Google Scholar, 32Benoit R.G. et al.Adaptive top-down suppression of hippocampal activity and the purging of intrusive memories from consciousness.J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015; 27: 96-111Crossref PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar, 33Depue B.E. et al.Prefrontal regions orchestrate suppression of emotional memories via a two-phase process.Science. 2007; 317: 215-219Crossref PubMed Scopus (257) Google Scholar, 34Depue B.E. et al.The organization of right prefrontal networks reveals common mechanisms of inhibitory regulation across cognitive, emotional, and motor processes.Cereb. Cortex. 2016; 26: 1634-1646Crossref PubMed Scopus (22) Google Scholar, 35van Schie K. Anderson M.C. Successfully controlling intrusive memories is harder when control must be sustained.Memory. 2017; 25: 1201-1216Crossref PubMed Scopus (1) Google Scholar]. DS begins when one encounters a reminder to an unwelcome memory or a thought. Such reminders are believed to trigger (owing to the affective qualities of the reminder, or of the unwelcome memory) inhibitory control to countermand the retrieval of the associated event, in a manner analogous to the way that control processes countermand motor processes to stop physical actions. Evidence indicates that repeated DS over multiple encounters with a reminder weakens the associated memory until it no longer intrudes [26Gagnepain P. et al.Parallel regulation of memory and emotion supports the suppression of intrusive memories.J. Neurosci. 2017; 37: 6423-6441Crossref PubMed Scopus (6) Google Scholar, 32Benoit R.G. et al.Adaptive top-down suppression of hippocampal activity and the purging of intrusive memories from consciousness.J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2015; 27: 96-111Crossref PubMed Scopus (0) Google Scholar, 36Levy B.J. Anderson M.C. Purging of memories from conscious awareness tracked in the human brain.J. Neurosci. 2012; 32: 16785-16794Crossref PubMed Scopus (47) Google Scholar, 37Hellerstedt R. et al.Tracking the intrusion of unwanted memories into awareness with event-related potentials.Neuropsychologia. 2016; 89: 510-523Crossref PubMed Scopus (6) Google Scholar], ultimately impairing voluntary retrieval of the memory, consistent with an active memory-inhibition mechanism [38Anderson M.C. Active forgetting: evidence for functional inhibition as a source of memory failure.J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma. 2001; 4: 185-210Crossref Scopus (0) Google Scholar]. Of course, memories vary in how amenable they are to suppression, and individuals vary in their ability to implement such suppression [29Levy B.J. Anderson M.C. Individual differences in the suppression of unwanted memories: the executive deficit hypothesis.Acta Psychol. 2008; 127: 623-635Crossref PubMed Scopus (126) Google Scholar]. Importantly, impaired DS is related to both subclinical traits associated with affective psychopa
最长约 10秒,即可获得该文献文件

科研通智能强力驱动
Strongly Powered by AbleSci AI
更新
大幅提高文件上传限制,最高150M (2024-4-1)

科研通是完全免费的文献互助平台,具备全网最快的应助速度,最高的求助完成率。 对每一个文献求助,科研通都将尽心尽力,给求助人一个满意的交代。
实时播报
思源应助高兴凝安采纳,获得10
5秒前
我是老大应助呈安一丁采纳,获得10
5秒前
6秒前
7秒前
Dr_Zhang关注了科研通微信公众号
11秒前
22秒前
alan完成签到 ,获得积分10
23秒前
doubleshake发布了新的文献求助10
24秒前
情怀应助doubleshake采纳,获得10
30秒前
88C真是太神奇啦完成签到 ,获得积分10
32秒前
wanci应助科研通管家采纳,获得10
38秒前
科研通AI2S应助科研通管家采纳,获得10
38秒前
39秒前
kouryoufu发布了新的文献求助10
44秒前
46秒前
LYF000666完成签到,获得积分10
46秒前
krajicek完成签到,获得积分10
57秒前
1分钟前
1分钟前
abiorz完成签到,获得积分10
1分钟前
buding给buding的求助进行了留言
1分钟前
窗外是蔚蓝色完成签到,获得积分10
1分钟前
Nick完成签到,获得积分10
1分钟前
1分钟前
拾溪完成签到,获得积分10
1分钟前
拾溪发布了新的文献求助10
1分钟前
满意人英完成签到,获得积分10
1分钟前
1分钟前
我是好好学习完成签到,获得积分10
2分钟前
giving完成签到 ,获得积分10
2分钟前
kouryoufu发布了新的文献求助10
2分钟前
2分钟前
2分钟前
吕半鬼完成签到,获得积分0
2分钟前
科目三应助摇摇奶昔采纳,获得10
2分钟前
英姑应助Nick采纳,获得10
2分钟前
2分钟前
Dr_Zhang发布了新的文献求助10
2分钟前
2分钟前
海派Hi完成签到 ,获得积分10
2分钟前
高分求助中
The late Devonian Standard Conodont Zonation 2000
Nickel superalloy market size, share, growth, trends, and forecast 2023-2030 2000
The Lali Section: An Excellent Reference Section for Upper - Devonian in South China 1500
Smart but Scattered: The Revolutionary Executive Skills Approach to Helping Kids Reach Their Potential (第二版) 1000
Very-high-order BVD Schemes Using β-variable THINC Method 850
Mantiden: Faszinierende Lauerjäger Faszinierende Lauerjäger 800
PraxisRatgeber: Mantiden: Faszinierende Lauerjäger 800
热门求助领域 (近24小时)
化学 医学 生物 材料科学 工程类 有机化学 生物化学 物理 内科学 纳米技术 计算机科学 化学工程 复合材料 基因 遗传学 催化作用 物理化学 免疫学 量子力学 细胞生物学
热门帖子
关注 科研通微信公众号,转发送积分 3252945
求助须知:如何正确求助?哪些是违规求助? 2895480
关于积分的说明 8286824
捐赠科研通 2564301
什么是DOI,文献DOI怎么找? 1392246
科研通“疑难数据库(出版商)”最低求助积分说明 652087
邀请新用户注册赠送积分活动 629383